Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ndmmz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-20T10:43:48.164Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

About me, you and her: Personal pronouns are developmentally preceded by mental state language

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 April 2022

Filip SMOLÍK*
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychology, Czech Academy of Sciences
Anna CHROMÁ
Affiliation:
Faculty of Arts, Charles University
*
Corresponding author. Filip Smolík, Institute of Psychology of the Czech Academy of Sciences, Hybernská 8, CZ-110 00 Praha 1, Czech Republic. E-mail: smolik@praha.psu.cas.cz

Abstract

Person-referring pronouns in the first and second person (I, your) have been viewed as signs of increasing social understanding in children due to their shifting reference properties. However, they are linguistically complex elements and might depend on general language development. We used longitudinal transcript data from Manchester corpus (12 children aged 2 to 3 years) to examine concurrent and predictive relations between pronouns, general language development (MLU), and social understanding (indexed by the use of mental state language). In the key analysis, social understanding but not general language was found to be a developmental precursor of first-, second- and third-person pronoun mastery. Results suggest that social understanding is needed for acquisition of all person reference, not only in first and second person. Results for first- and third-person pronouns were more similar than for second person, suggesting that social-cognitive demands of person reference go beyond shifting reference of first- and second-person.

Type
Article
Copyright
© Institute of Psychology CAS, Czech Republic, 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth EditionCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barokova, M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2020). Person-reference in autism spectrum disorder: Developmental trends and the role of linguistic input. Autism Research, 13(6), 959969.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. M. (1995). Children talk about the mind. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bates, D., Maechler, M., & Bolker, B. (2011). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using {S4} classes.Google Scholar
Beeghly, M., Bretherton, I., & Mervis, C. B. (1986). Mothers’ internal state language to toddlers. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4(3), 247261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beeghly, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1994). Child maltreatment, attachment, and the self system: Emergence of an internal state lexicon in toddlers at high social risk. Development and Psychopathology, 6(1), 530.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bretherton, I., & Beeghly, M. (1982). Talking about internal states: The acquisition of an explicit theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 18, 906921.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, J. R., Donelan-McCall, N., & Dunn, J. (1996). Why talk about mental states? The significance of children’s conversations with friends, siblings, and mothers. Child Development, 67(3), 836849.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brownell, C. A., Ramani, G. B., & Zerwas, S. (2006). Becoming a Social Partner With Peers: Cooperation and Social Understanding in One-and Two-Year-Olds. Child Development, 77(4), 803821.Google ScholarPubMed
Charney, R. (1980). Pronoun errors in autistic children: Support for a social explanation. British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 15(1), 3943.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiat, S. (1982). If I were you and you were me: The analysis of pronouns in a pronoun-reversing child. Journal of Child Language, 9(2), 359379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chromá, A., & Smolík, F. (2017). Personal Pronouns and Verb Person Inflections: Relations with Linguistic Development and Social Understanding. In Scott, M. L. & Jennifer (Eds.), Proceedings of the 41st annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 168178). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Courage, M. L., Edison, S. C., & Howe, M. L. (2004). Variability in the early development of visual self-recognition. Infant Behavior and Development, 27, 509532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dale, P. S., & Crain-Thoreson, C. (1993). Pronoun reversals: Who, when, and why? Journal of Child Language, 20, 573589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, J., Brown, J., Slomkowski, C., Tesla, C., & Youngblade, L. (1991). Young children’s understanding of other people’s feelings and beliefs: Individual differences and their antecedents. Child Development, 62(6), 13521366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, K. E., & Demuth, K. (2012). Individual differences in pronoun reversal: Evidence from two longitudinal case studies. Journal of Child Language, 39(1), 162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, J., & Weisberg, S. (2018). An R companion to applied regression. Sage publications.Google Scholar
Gernsbacher, M. A., Morson, E. M., & Grace, E. J. (2016). Language and Speech in Autism. Annual review of linguistics, 2, 413–425. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-linguist-030514-124824CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Girouard, P. C., Ricard, M., & Decarie, T. G. (1997). The acquisition of personal pronouns in French-speaking and English-speaking children. Journal of Child Language, 24(2), 311326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodenough, F. L. (1938). The use of pronouns by young children: A note on the development of self-awareness. The Pedagogical Seminary and Journal of Genetic Psychology, 52(2), 333346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J., & Hedges, L. V. (2010). Sources of variability in children’s language growth. Cognitive Psychology, 61(4), 343365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jordan, R. R. (1989). An experimental comparison of the understanding and use of speaker-addressee personal pronouns in autistic children. British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 24(2), 169179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217250.Google Scholar
Kelty-Stephen, E., Fein, D. A., & Naigles, L. R. (2020). Children with ASD use joint attention and linguistic skill in pronoun development. Language Acquisition, 124.Google ScholarPubMed
Kristen, S., Sodian, B., Thoermer, C., & Perst, H. (2011). Infants’ joint attention skills predict toddlers’ emerging mental state language. Developmental Psychology, 47(5), 1207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13). https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v082.i13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, M., & Ramsay, D. (2004). Development of self-recognition, personal pronoun use, and pretend play during the 2nd year. Child Development, 75, 18211831.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Longobardi, E., Spataro, P., Pecora, G., & Bellagamba, F. (2019). Indices of social knowledge in Italian-speaking children: Relations between personal pronouns, verb conjugations and mental state language. First Language, 39(4), 396–411. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723719840514CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loveland, K. (1984). Learning about points of view: Spatial perspective and the acquisition of “I/you.” Journal of Child Langauge, 11, 535556.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk. Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Markova, G., & Smolík, F. (2014). What do you think? The relationship between person reference and communication about the mind in toddlers. Social Development, 23(1), 6179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, C., Pure, K., & Furrow, D. (1990). Children’s understanding of the modal expression of speaker certainty and uncertainty and its relation to the development of a representational theory of mind. Child Development, 61(3), 722730.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Naigles, L. R., Cheng, M., Xu Rattanasone, N., Tek, S., Khetrapal, N., Fein, D., & Demuth, K. (2016). “You’re telling me!” The prevalence and predictors of pronoun reversals in children with autism spectrum disorders and typical development. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 27, 11–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2016.03.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oshima-Takane, Y. (1992). Analysis of pronominal errors: A case-study. Journal of Child Language, 19(1), 111131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Overweg, J., Hartman, C. A., & Hendriks, P. (2018). Children with autism spectrum disorder show pronoun reversals in interpretation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 127(2), 228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piaget, J. (1929). The Child’s Conception of the World. Routledge & K. Paul.Google Scholar
Rowland, C. F., Pine, J. M., Lieven, E. V. M., & Theakston, A. L. (2003). Determinants of Acquisition Order in Wh-Questions: Re-Evaluating the Role of Caregiver Speech. Journal of Child Language, 30, 609635.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schiff-Myers, N. B. (1983). From pronoun reversals to correct pronoun usage: A case study of a normally developing child. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 48(4), 394402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shatz, M., Wellman, H. M., & Silber, S. (1983). The acquisition of mental verbs: A systematic investigation of the first reference to mental state. Cognition, 14(3), 301321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smiley, P. A., Chang, L. K., & Allhoff, A. K. (2011). Can Toddy give me an orange? Parent input and young children’s production of I and you. Language Learning and Development, 7(2), 77106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smolík, F., & Bláhová, V. (2021). Early linguistic reference to first and second person depends on social understanding as well as language skills: Evidence from Czech 30-month-olds. First Language, 41(1), 109125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Szagun, G., & Schramm, S. A. (2019). Lexically driven or early structure building? Constructing an early grammar in German child language. First Language, 39(1), 6179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1994). Constraints on language acquisition: Studies of atypical children. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Wechsler, S. (2010). What “you” and “I” mean to each other: Person indexicals, self-ascription, and theory of mind. Language, 86(2), 332365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655684.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed